The classification of aphasias: A brief history (notice n° 222897)

détails MARC
000 -LEADER
fixed length control field 03863cam a2200217 4500500
005 - DATE AND TIME OF LATEST TRANSACTION
control field 20250112060505.0
041 ## - LANGUAGE CODE
Language code of text/sound track or separate title fre
042 ## - AUTHENTICATION CODE
Authentication code dc
100 10 - MAIN ENTRY--PERSONAL NAME
Personal name Viader, Fausto
Relator term author
245 00 - TITLE STATEMENT
Title The classification of aphasias: A brief history
260 ## - PUBLICATION, DISTRIBUTION, ETC.
Date of publication, distribution, etc. 2015.<br/>
500 ## - GENERAL NOTE
General note 67
520 ## - SUMMARY, ETC.
Summary, etc. The variety of aphasia types was immediately sensed by Paul Broca himself, shortly after he had described his first case. He then distinguished “alogia,” “verbal amnesia,” “aphemia,” and “mechanic alalia.” The classification of aphasias has ever since appeared as an obvious need to generations of neurologists. In 1874, Carl Wernicke was the first to propose a classification of aphasias based on the anatomical correspondence of the various aspects of language impairment, with a posterior center and an anterior center connected by an anatomical pathway, the damage to which would give rise to sensory, motor, and conduction aphasia, respectively. This concept was further elaborated, to culminate in the well-known Lichtheim's “house” scheme. In addition to the three aforementioned types, this model included “transcortical” (motor, sensory, or mixed) and “pure” (caused by impairment of a single linguistic ability, e.g. agraphia, alexia, or pure verbal deafness) aphasias. This view was supported by generations of neurologists throughout the twentieth century, from Jules Dejerine to Norman Geschwind, despite strong criticism from advocates of the holistic conception of language, such as Pierre Marie or Kurt Goldstein, who did not agree with the idea that language could be split into multiple functions, which in turn would depend on multiple cerebral structures. As Pierre Marie put it, “There is only one aphasia, Wernicke's, and Broca's aphasia is nothing but Wernicke's aphasia without speech.” Despite occasional attempts to modify it, the “connexionist” model came to us virtually unchanged, and is still present in neurology textbooks. The emergence of CT and MRI scans from the 1980s on brought about new concepts, e.g. “subcortical aphasias,” which were called “dissident” because they did not fit the clinical profile of classical aphasic syndromes. This is an example of how better anatomical lesion analysis can help to refine the clinical approach. A completely new era in aphasia classification dawned with the intervention of, first, linguistics and, second, cognitive neuropsychology. The linguist Roman Jakobson stressed the importance of the paradigmatic/syntagmatic dichotomy, corresponding to selection and combination, respectively, of linguistic elements (phonemes/morphemes/words). The target of the linguistic analysis is only the language disorder, regardless of any anatomical correlation. Cognitive neuropsychology individualizes cognitive syndromes with the aim of finding double dissociations in order to infer modular models of normal cerebral functioning. Contrary to pure linguistics, cognitive neuropsychology (and therefore neuropsycholinguistics) aims at uncovering the brain areas that support specific speech processes. In the field of aphasia, some well-known achievements of neuropsycholinguistics include reading and writing disorders such as deep and surface dyslexia, for example. Although the ancient classifications inherited from the nineteenth century are still in use in everyday neurology, the continuous advances in brain imaging, together with the new methods of linguistic analysis, will no doubt result in further refinements in aphasia nosology.
690 ## - LOCAL SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM (OCLC, RLIN)
Topical term or geographic name as entry element aphasia
690 ## - LOCAL SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM (OCLC, RLIN)
Topical term or geographic name as entry element history
690 ## - LOCAL SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM (OCLC, RLIN)
Topical term or geographic name as entry element aphasiology
690 ## - LOCAL SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM (OCLC, RLIN)
Topical term or geographic name as entry element language
690 ## - LOCAL SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM (OCLC, RLIN)
Topical term or geographic name as entry element linguistic
786 0# - DATA SOURCE ENTRY
Note Revue de neuropsychologie | Volume 7 | 1 | 2015-04-07 | p. 5-14 | 2101-6739
856 41 - ELECTRONIC LOCATION AND ACCESS
Uniform Resource Identifier <a href="https://shs.cairn.info/journal-revue-de-neuropsychologie-2015-1-page-5?lang=en">https://shs.cairn.info/journal-revue-de-neuropsychologie-2015-1-page-5?lang=en</a>

Pas d'exemplaire disponible.

PLUDOC

PLUDOC est la plateforme unique et centralisée de gestion des bibliothèques physiques et numériques de Guinée administré par le CEDUST. Elle est la plus grande base de données de ressources documentaires pour les Étudiants, Enseignants chercheurs et Chercheurs de Guinée.

Adresse

627 919 101/664 919 101

25 boulevard du commerce
Kaloum, Conakry, Guinée

Réseaux sociaux

Powered by Netsen Group @ 2025